Overview

Nel nostro modo di usare le parole, da quelle pi? nobili a quelle pi? triviali, ? racchiusa una teoria dello spazio e del tempo, della materia e della causalit?. Nel linguaggio, sia colto sia quotidiano, sono radicati un modello (anzi, due) di sessualit?, i concetti di divinit?, potere e giustizia, le nozioni di intimit?, benessere e pericolo, una filosofia del libero arbitrio. E poich?, al di l? di minime variazioni, la logica d'insieme ? la stessa per ogni lingua, un esame approfondito dei mezzi e delle ...
See more details below
Fatti di parole

Available on NOOK devices and apps  
  • NOOK Devices
  • NOOK HD/HD+ Tablet
  • NOOK
  • NOOK Color
  • NOOK Tablet
  • Tablet/Phone
  • NOOK for Windows 8 Tablet
  • NOOK for iOS
  • NOOK for Android
  • NOOK Kids for iPad
  • PC/Mac
  • NOOK for Windows 8
  • NOOK for PC
  • NOOK for Mac
  • NOOK Study
  • NOOK for Web

Want a NOOK? Explore Now

NOOK Book (eBook)
$13.99
BN.com price

Overview

Nel nostro modo di usare le parole, da quelle più nobili a quelle più triviali, è racchiusa una teoria dello spazio e del tempo, della materia e della causalità. Nel linguaggio, sia colto sia quotidiano, sono radicati un modello (anzi, due) di sessualità, i concetti di divinità, potere e giustizia, le nozioni di intimità, benessere e pericolo, una filosofia del libero arbitrio. E poiché, al di là di minime variazioni, la logica d'insieme è la stessa per ogni lingua, un esame approfondito dei mezzi e delle modalità di comunicazione verbale - dai discorsi volti a informare o a convincere, a minacciare o a sedurre, alle imprecazioni, ai nomi che scegliamo per i figli - può rivelarsi prezioso per capire chi siamo e le vere ragioni dei nostri comportamenti. È l'obiettivo che si pone Steven Pinker in Fatti di parole , in cui confluiscono i due fecondi itinerari di ricerca da lui percorsi negli ultimi anni: lo studio della facoltà di linguaggio e l'indagine sui processi cognitivi e sulla natura umana. In questa prospettiva l'autore si propone di analizzare il significato di parole e frasi in contesti sociali, ovvero nelle varie occasioni in cui cerchiamo di esprimere verbalmente i nostri pensieri e sentimenti. La conclusione a cui giunge è che ogni essere umano si forma un'immagine del mondo fisico e del mondo sociale che non corrisponde al flusso di sensazioni provocato dall'incontro della mente con l'ambiente esterno, ma è costruita a partire da un repertorio limitato e identificabile di pensieri primari, o meglio di «concetti naturali» (per esempio, spazio, forza, dominanza, parentela e contaminazione), che, grazie alla loro plasticità e valenza metaforica, sono in grado di ricomporre i dati d'esperienza in una molteplicità di oggetti ed eventi traducibili in vocaboli e proposizioni. La prevalenza di alcuni di questi «manufatti mentali» (nomi, parole, stringhe linguistiche) in quella rete di influenze reciproche che è la comunità dei parlanti, dove ogni individuo è insieme produttore e consumatore di significati, definisce ciò che chiamiamo «cultura» di una società, di cui la lingua è parte integrante. Trattando con grande chiarezza e rigore scientifico idee complesse e originali, come di consueto Pinker non rinuncia al suo stile ricco di elegante ironia, facendo spesso ricorso a giochi di parole e a esempi tratti dall'attualità, dalla letteratura di consumo e da film, fumetti e videogiochi, quasi che solo un approccio di divertita leggerezza possa strappare al linguaggio i segreti dell'ancora misterioso funzionamento della mente e, quindi, della natura umana.
Read More Show Less

Product Details

  • ISBN-13: 9788852015441
  • Publisher: MONDADORI
  • Publication date: 10/7/2010
  • Language: Italian
  • Sold by: ARNOLDO MONDADORI - EBKS
  • Format: eBook
  • File size: 2 MB

Meet the Author

Steven Pinker
Steven Pinker
Besides challenging conventional wisdom about how we think, cognitive scientist Steven Pinker has a talent for conveying his findings about the brain, language and perception with a clarity and cleverness that has brought him a following outside his field.

Biography

"When a gifted scientist and a gifted writer are all in one, you have Steven Pinker," writes fellow cognitive scientist Michael S. Gazzaniga. With his crisp prose style and zany, pop culture-inflected sense of humor, the MIT psychology professor has become famed for his ability to turn something like a discussion of regular and irregular verb forms into a rollicking good read.

As a psychology student at McGill University in Montreal, Pinker was drawn to the emergent field of cognitive science: "I found alluring the combination of psychology, computer science, artificial intelligence, the philosophy of mind, and linguistics," he said in a Scientific American interview. He earned his Ph.D. at Harvard, where his mentor was the psychology professor Roger Brown, who was a pioneer in the study of language acquisition and one of the first to apply Noam Chomsky's theories of language to field research. After accepting a post at MIT in 1982, Pinker began studying language acquisition in children, amassing enough data to demonstrate that children have an inborn facility for language.

Pinker's academic works on language development were admired by many of his peers, but in 1994 he sought—and gained—a broader audience with The Language Instinct, which suggests that human language is a biological adaptation, like web-spinning in spiders, rather than (as it is sometimes seen) a cultural invention, like the wheel. Pinker's lively and engaging treatise held tremendous appeal for a popular audience. Michael Coe, writing in The New York Times, called The Language Instinct "A brilliant, witty and altogether satisfying book."

But if humans have an instinct for language, how was that instinct acquired? That question led Pinker to the field of evolutionary psychology, and to the writing of his next book, How the Mind Works. If a particular behavior is common among humans, evolutionary psychologists reason, that behavior probably contributed to the ability of earlier humans to survive and pass along their genes. How the Mind Works, which uses this approach to examine behaviors from music-making to murder, was a finalist for the 1998 Pulitzer Prize and the National Book Award. Following its release, Pinker publicly tangled with Stephen Jay Gould over the scientific legitimacy of evolutionary psychology. Although the two scientists clashed on some issues, Pinker admired Gould's ability to write entertaining explications of complex ideas—"profundity with a light touch," as Pinker wrote in his Time magazine eulogy for Gould.

Pinker's next book, Words and Rules, returned to the subject of language; specifically, it explores the different mechanisms involved in learning regular and irregular verb forms. In a recent book The Blank Slate, Pinker tackled the objections some people have to a biological view of human nature. "There are fears that if you acknowledge that people are born with anything, it implies that some people have more of it than others, and therefore it would open the door to political inequality or oppression, for example," he explained in a New York Times interview. The Blank Slate is Pinker's attempt to demonstrate that there's no inherent contradiction between evolutionary psychology and the concepts of free will and moral behavior. "It's a fallacy to think that hunger and thirst and a sex drive are biological but that reasoning and decision making and learning are something else, something non-biological," he said. "They're just a different kind of biology."

Good To Know

Journalists often comment on Pinker's rock-star mane of curls, and indeed Pinker once flirted with the idea of becoming a rock musician: "I have to confess that watching rock 'n' roll concerts, I did fantasize about being up on stage," he told The Guardian. "Not in the lead. I never wanted to be Mick Jagger. Maybe the bass-player or the drummer. But I never, ever played air guitar."

Research at Pinker's lab, in the Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences at MIT, focuses on the different mental processes involved in using grammatical rules (e.g., an English plural can be formed by adding –s to the end of a noun) and using exceptions to the rules (e.g., the plural of mouse is not mouses but mice). The lab has undertaken magnetoencephalographic (MEG) studies to identify "the time course of the processing of words and rules in the brain."

Pinker was named among Newsweek's "100 Americans for the Next Century" and included in Esquire's "Register of Outstanding Men and Women."

Read More Show Less
    1. Hometown:
      Boston, Massachusetts
    1. Date of Birth:
      September 18, 1954
    2. Place of Birth:
      Montreal, Canada
    1. Education:
      B.A., McGill University, 1976; Ph.D., Harvard University, 1979
    2. Website:

Customer Reviews

Be the first to write a review
( 0 )
Rating Distribution

5 Star

(0)

4 Star

(0)

3 Star

(0)

2 Star

(0)

1 Star

(0)

Your Rating:

Your Name: Create a Pen Name or

Barnes & Noble.com Review Rules

Our reader reviews allow you to share your comments on titles you liked, or didn't, with others. By submitting an online review, you are representing to Barnes & Noble.com that all information contained in your review is original and accurate in all respects, and that the submission of such content by you and the posting of such content by Barnes & Noble.com does not and will not violate the rights of any third party. Please follow the rules below to help ensure that your review can be posted.

Reviews by Our Customers Under the Age of 13

We highly value and respect everyone's opinion concerning the titles we offer. However, we cannot allow persons under the age of 13 to have accounts at BN.com or to post customer reviews. Please see our Terms of Use for more details.

What to exclude from your review:

Please do not write about reviews, commentary, or information posted on the product page. If you see any errors in the information on the product page, please send us an email.

Reviews should not contain any of the following:

  • - HTML tags, profanity, obscenities, vulgarities, or comments that defame anyone
  • - Time-sensitive information such as tour dates, signings, lectures, etc.
  • - Single-word reviews. Other people will read your review to discover why you liked or didn't like the title. Be descriptive.
  • - Comments focusing on the author or that may ruin the ending for others
  • - Phone numbers, addresses, URLs
  • - Pricing and availability information or alternative ordering information
  • - Advertisements or commercial solicitation

Reminder:

  • - By submitting a review, you grant to Barnes & Noble.com and its sublicensees the royalty-free, perpetual, irrevocable right and license to use the review in accordance with the Barnes & Noble.com Terms of Use.
  • - Barnes & Noble.com reserves the right not to post any review -- particularly those that do not follow the terms and conditions of these Rules. Barnes & Noble.com also reserves the right to remove any review at any time without notice.
  • - See Terms of Use for other conditions and disclaimers.
Search for Products You'd Like to Recommend

Recommend other products that relate to your review. Just search for them below and share!

Create a Pen Name

Your Pen Name is your unique identity on BN.com. It will appear on the reviews you write and other website activities. Your Pen Name cannot be edited, changed or deleted once submitted.

 
Your Pen Name can be any combination of alphanumeric characters (plus - and _), and must be at least two characters long.

Continue Anonymously

    If you find inappropriate content, please report it to Barnes & Noble
    Why is this product inappropriate?
    Comments (optional)