Function, Visualization, And Mathematical Thinking For College Students With Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder.

Paperback (Print)
Buy New
Buy New from BN.com
$62.02
Used and New from Other Sellers
Used and New from Other Sellers
from $72.23
Usually ships in 1-2 business days
Other sellers (Paperback)
  • All (2) from $72.23   
  • New (2) from $72.23   

More About This Textbook

Overview

This qualitative study investigated the nature of mathematical thinking in learning the function concept among three college students with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). The theoretical framework for this study combined ideas from Social Cognitive Theory (Bandura, 2001), Barkley's Unifying Theory of ADHD (1997) and work on covariational reasoning by (Carlson, Jacobs, Coe, Larson, & Hsu, 2002). Primary data were tutoring sessions in mathematics with three ADHD college students at two western universities over the course of one semester. The results presented here give a picture of these students' cognition around the concept of function. In addition, I compared ADHD and non---ADHD college students' thinking during a problem solving situation calling for the generation of a graph of height as a function of volume. Two ADHD college student participants, two college student participants without ADHD, and two instructors of mathematics completed the same task, the Bottle Problem as used by Carlson and colleagues (2002). I analyzed their responses and compared them to reported responses of participants in Carlson et al. The results of the tutoring sessions and the bottle problem interviews indicate that there are similarities and differences between the difficulties ADHD college mathematics students encounter and those outlined in published literature for all college students learning mathematics. Similarities include sparse concept image of what constitutes a function and difficulty making connections among graphical, tabular, symbolic, and verbal representations of functions. However, there are differences in the types of representations each group works with most flexibly. The literature reports that college students, in general, express a preference for symbolic representations over graphical ones. In contrast, the results of this study suggest that working with visually rich representations, particularly graphical and tabular ones, may be a strength for ADHD college students. Moreover, a theory-building results of the dissertation is a refinement I made to the concept-level model introduced by Carlson et al. in the context of the bottle problem. I expanded the applicability of this model by connecting it to a larger cognitive framework for mathematical problem solving based on Barkley's (1998) unified theory of ADHD.
Read More Show Less

Product Details

  • ISBN-13: 9781243980083
  • Publisher: BiblioLabsII
  • Publication date: 9/10/2011
  • Pages: 274
  • Product dimensions: 7.44 (w) x 9.69 (h) x 0.58 (d)

Customer Reviews

Be the first to write a review
( 0 )
Rating Distribution

5 Star

(0)

4 Star

(0)

3 Star

(0)

2 Star

(0)

1 Star

(0)

Your Rating:

Your Name: Create a Pen Name or

Barnes & Noble.com Review Rules

Our reader reviews allow you to share your comments on titles you liked, or didn't, with others. By submitting an online review, you are representing to Barnes & Noble.com that all information contained in your review is original and accurate in all respects, and that the submission of such content by you and the posting of such content by Barnes & Noble.com does not and will not violate the rights of any third party. Please follow the rules below to help ensure that your review can be posted.

Reviews by Our Customers Under the Age of 13

We highly value and respect everyone's opinion concerning the titles we offer. However, we cannot allow persons under the age of 13 to have accounts at BN.com or to post customer reviews. Please see our Terms of Use for more details.

What to exclude from your review:

Please do not write about reviews, commentary, or information posted on the product page. If you see any errors in the information on the product page, please send us an email.

Reviews should not contain any of the following:

  • - HTML tags, profanity, obscenities, vulgarities, or comments that defame anyone
  • - Time-sensitive information such as tour dates, signings, lectures, etc.
  • - Single-word reviews. Other people will read your review to discover why you liked or didn't like the title. Be descriptive.
  • - Comments focusing on the author or that may ruin the ending for others
  • - Phone numbers, addresses, URLs
  • - Pricing and availability information or alternative ordering information
  • - Advertisements or commercial solicitation

Reminder:

  • - By submitting a review, you grant to Barnes & Noble.com and its sublicensees the royalty-free, perpetual, irrevocable right and license to use the review in accordance with the Barnes & Noble.com Terms of Use.
  • - Barnes & Noble.com reserves the right not to post any review -- particularly those that do not follow the terms and conditions of these Rules. Barnes & Noble.com also reserves the right to remove any review at any time without notice.
  • - See Terms of Use for other conditions and disclaimers.
Search for Products You'd Like to Recommend

Recommend other products that relate to your review. Just search for them below and share!

Create a Pen Name

Your Pen Name is your unique identity on BN.com. It will appear on the reviews you write and other website activities. Your Pen Name cannot be edited, changed or deleted once submitted.

 
Your Pen Name can be any combination of alphanumeric characters (plus - and _), and must be at least two characters long.

Continue Anonymously

    If you find inappropriate content, please report it to Barnes & Noble
    Why is this product inappropriate?
    Comments (optional)