BN.com Gift Guide

The Subjectivity of Scientists and the Bayesian Approach / Edition 1

Hardcover (Print)
Used and New from Other Sellers
Used and New from Other Sellers
from $33.65
Usually ships in 1-2 business days
(Save 77%)
Other sellers (Hardcover)
  • All (6) from $33.65   
  • New (3) from $109.49   
  • Used (3) from $33.65   
Close
Sort by
Page 1 of 1
Showing All
Note: Marketplace items are not eligible for any BN.com coupons and promotions
$109.49
Seller since 2010

Feedback rating:

(33)

Condition:

New — never opened or used in original packaging.

Like New — packaging may have been opened. A "Like New" item is suitable to give as a gift.

Very Good — may have minor signs of wear on packaging but item works perfectly and has no damage.

Good — item is in good condition but packaging may have signs of shelf wear/aging or torn packaging. All specific defects should be noted in the Comments section associated with each item.

Acceptable — item is in working order but may show signs of wear such as scratches or torn packaging. All specific defects should be noted in the Comments section associated with each item.

Used — An item that has been opened and may show signs of wear. All specific defects should be noted in the Comments section associated with each item.

Refurbished — A used item that has been renewed or updated and verified to be in proper working condition. Not necessarily completed by the original manufacturer.

New
0471396850 HARDCOVER. NEW. Never used. We do not ship to PO Box/APO/FPO addresses.

Ships from: Cupertino, CA

Usually ships in 1-2 business days

  • Canadian
  • International
  • Standard, 48 States
  • Standard (AK, HI)
  • Express, 48 States
  • Express (AK, HI)
$113.69
Seller since 2008

Feedback rating:

(4532)

Condition: New
New Book. Shipped from UK within 4 to 14 business days. Established seller since 2000.

Ships from: Horcott Rd, Fairford, United Kingdom

Usually ships in 1-2 business days

  • Standard, 48 States
  • Standard (AK, HI)
$157.87
Seller since 2008

Feedback rating:

(196)

Condition: New

Ships from: Chicago, IL

Usually ships in 1-2 business days

  • Standard, 48 States
  • Standard (AK, HI)
Page 1 of 1
Showing All
Close
Sort by

Overview

Comparing and contrasting the reality of subjectivity in the work of history's great scientists and the modern Bayesian approach to statistical analysis
Scientists and researchers are taught to analyze their data from an objective point of view, allowing the data to speak for themselves rather than assigning them meaning based on expectations or opinions. But scientists have never behaved fully objectively. Throughout history, some of our greatest scientific minds have relied on intuition, hunches, and personal beliefs to make sense of empirical data-and these subjective influences have often aided in humanity's greatest scientific achievements. The authors argue that subjectivity has not only played a significant role in the advancement of science, but that science will advance more rapidly if the modern methods of Bayesian statistical analysis replace some of the classical twentieth-century methods that have traditionally been taught.
To accomplish this goal, the authors examine the lives and work of history's great scientists and show that even the most successful have sometimes misrepresented findings or been influenced by their own preconceived notions of religion, metaphysics, and the occult, or the personal beliefs of their mentors. Contrary to popular belief, our greatest scientific thinkers approached their data with a combination of subjectivity and empiricism, and thus informally achieved what is more formally accomplished by the modern Bayesian approach to data analysis.
Yet we are still taught that science is purely objective. This innovative book dispels that myth using historical accounts and biographical sketches of more than a dozen great scientists, including Aristotle, Galileo Galilei, Johannes Kepler, William Harvey, Sir Isaac Newton, Antoine Levoisier, Alexander von Humboldt, Michael Faraday, Charles Darwin, Louis Pasteur, Gregor Mendel, Sigmund Freud, Marie Curie, Robert Millikan, Albert Einstein, Sir Cyril Burt, and Margaret Mead. Also included is a detailed treatment of the modern Bayesian approach to data analysis. Up-to-date references to the Bayesian theoretical and applied literature, as well as reference lists of the primary sources of the principal works of all the scientists discussed, round out this comprehensive treatment of the subject.
Readers will benefit from this cogent and enlightening view of the history of subjectivity in science and the authors' alternative vision of how the Bayesian approach should be used to further the cause of science and learning well into the twenty-first century.

Read More Show Less

Editorial Reviews

From the Publisher
"Press and Tanur argue that subjectivity has not only played a significant role in the advancement of science, but that science will advance more rapidly if the modern methods of Bayesian statistical analysis replace some of the more classical twentieth-century methods." (SciTech Book News, Vol. 25, No. 3, September 2001)

"An insightful work." (Choice, Vol. 39, No. 4, December 2001)

"compilation of interesting and popular problems" (Short Book Reviews - Publication of the Int. Statistical Institute, December 2001)

"...this book is fascinating." (Short Book Reviews, Vol. 21, No. 3, December 2001)

"...highlight the role of subjectivity in science by describing the life and works of 17 scientists." (Zentralblatt MATH, Vol. 973, 2001/23)

Booknews
Press (University of California, Riverside) and Tanur (State University of New York, Stony Brook) argue that subjectivity has not only played a significant role in the advancement of science, but that science will advance more rapidly if the modern methods of Bayesian statistical analysis replace some of the more classical twentieth- century methods. To make their case, they examine the lives and work of influential scientists, and show that even the most successful have sometimes misrepresented findings or been influenced by their own preconceived notions of religion, metaphysics, and the occult, or the personal beliefs of their mentors. Among the scientists discussed are Aristotle, Galileo, Kepler, Newton, Faraday, Darwin, Pasteur, Mendel, Freud, Curie, Einstein, and Mead. Annotation c. Book News, Inc., Portland, OR (booknews.com)
Read More Show Less

Product Details

Meet the Author

S. JAMES PRESS is Distinguished Professor in the Department of Statistics at the University of California at Riverside. He is also the author of Bayesian Statistics: Principles, Models, and Applications (Wiley) and Applied Multivariate Analysis: Including Bayesian and Frequentist Methods of Inference.
JUDITH M. TANUR is Distinguished Teaching Professor in the Department of Sociology at the State University of New York at Stony Brook. She is also the editor of Statistics: A Guide to the Unknown and Questions About Questions: Inquiries into the Cognitive Bases of Surveys.

Read More Show Less

Read an Excerpt

Chapter 1: Introduction

This is a book about science, about scientists, and about the methods that scientists use. We will show that the most famous scientists in history have all used their hunches, beliefs, intuition, and deep understanding of the processes they studied, to one extent or another, to arrive at their conclusions. The reader will see that the oft-expressed notion that science is "objective" is only partially true; in fact, science is really a combination of both subjective and objective views.

In this book we tell the stories of 12 of the most famous scientists throughout history, from Aristotle, the philosopher who lived during the era of the ancient Greeks, to Albert Einstein, who lived during the twentieth century. In each case, we discuss how the scientist's preconceived beliefs informally influenced his or her scientific conclusions.

In Chapter 2 we explain that we did not choose these particular 12 scientists to study; they were selected for us. In Chapter 3 we tell the stories of five other very celebrated cases of famous scientists who may have stepped over the lines of acceptable scientific practice. Such overstepping arose either because their convictions about the correctness of their ideas led them to see or accept as accurate only what their theory predicted, or from their zeal to convince the world of the scientific merits of their work. A major portion of the book is devoted to the stories of our 12 most famous scientists in history (Chapter 4). We examine the lives of these people, their scientific contributions, and the ways in which they used their beliefs together with the results of their scientific experiments to carry out their research. In a final section of Chapter 4 we conjecture about what these scientists might have done had modern methods been available to them for combining their preconceptions about the processes they were studying with their experimental data.

Finally, in Chapter 5, we examine how subjectivity is being used in science in modern times. That discussion focuses on Bayesian statistical .science. But first, in this introductory chapter, we provide some definitions and background for what is to come.

Because we will be using the words subjectivity and objectivity frequently in this book, we must first explain how we arc using these terms. We intend to use them in several ways. But we will always be thinking about observational data, about the distinction between beliefs held by a scientist about a phenomenon prior to collecting the data, and beliefs held by the scientist after the data have been collected and analyzed.

In common usage, certain entities are seen to have only a subjective reality, in that those entities are constructs based on views and beliefs that were formed out the human mind. On the other hand, the term objective reality is used for entities that exist outside the minds of individuals, in that they exist in the world regardless of whether a person perceives them. For example, the opinions people hold about a political or social issue are personal beliefs that have only a subjective reality, whereas our starting point with respect to external reality is that the moon, the sun, and the planets would all exist regardless of whether or not human beings perceived them. But objective entities need not be corporeal. For example, all the scientific laws that govern the behavior of the physical world would exist regardless of their codification by human beings or humans' belief in them. The crux of our usage makes these terms specific to the scientific endeavor. When anything is observed or measured by human beings, human perception and sensory mechanisms are involved, and the resulting observations that are collected then depend on subjectively based distortions of objective reality. Further, after entities in the objective world are observed (perceived) by a human being, either through the senses and/or assisted by measuring instruments, the resulting measurements (called data) are interpreted by a human being in a subjective way that reflects the person's own experience, understanding, and preconceived notions and beliefs about the entities, the object, phenomenon, or construct being measured. The interpretation of such data is also affected by the person's state of mind and state of senses at the time of the interpretation. We use the term subjective, or subjectivity, to refer to preexisting views or beliefs about entities that influence both the gathering of data and their interpretation.

Broadening our interpretation of the term, we will also use subjectivity to mean a person's intuition, belief, and understanding about some proposition or hypothesis prior to that person collecting observational data that bear on the proposition, or prior to that person obtaining information about the hypothesis. Because the views about the hypothesis that we are talking about are those that a particular person held prior to that person having collected data, or they are his or her views prior to being told about the values of data that have been collected, and because these views may well differ across individuals, we call those views or beliefs subjective...

Read More Show Less

Table of Contents

Preface.

Introduction.

Selecting the Scientists.

Some Well-Known Stories of Extreme Subjectivity.

Stories of Famous Scientists.

Subjectivity in Science in Modern Times: The Bayesian Approach.

Appendix: References by Field of Application for Bayesian Statistical Science.

Bibliography.

Subject Index.

Name Index.

Read More Show Less

Customer Reviews

Be the first to write a review
( 0 )
Rating Distribution

5 Star

(0)

4 Star

(0)

3 Star

(0)

2 Star

(0)

1 Star

(0)

Your Rating:

Your Name: Create a Pen Name or

Barnes & Noble.com Review Rules

Our reader reviews allow you to share your comments on titles you liked, or didn't, with others. By submitting an online review, you are representing to Barnes & Noble.com that all information contained in your review is original and accurate in all respects, and that the submission of such content by you and the posting of such content by Barnes & Noble.com does not and will not violate the rights of any third party. Please follow the rules below to help ensure that your review can be posted.

Reviews by Our Customers Under the Age of 13

We highly value and respect everyone's opinion concerning the titles we offer. However, we cannot allow persons under the age of 13 to have accounts at BN.com or to post customer reviews. Please see our Terms of Use for more details.

What to exclude from your review:

Please do not write about reviews, commentary, or information posted on the product page. If you see any errors in the information on the product page, please send us an email.

Reviews should not contain any of the following:

  • - HTML tags, profanity, obscenities, vulgarities, or comments that defame anyone
  • - Time-sensitive information such as tour dates, signings, lectures, etc.
  • - Single-word reviews. Other people will read your review to discover why you liked or didn't like the title. Be descriptive.
  • - Comments focusing on the author or that may ruin the ending for others
  • - Phone numbers, addresses, URLs
  • - Pricing and availability information or alternative ordering information
  • - Advertisements or commercial solicitation

Reminder:

  • - By submitting a review, you grant to Barnes & Noble.com and its sublicensees the royalty-free, perpetual, irrevocable right and license to use the review in accordance with the Barnes & Noble.com Terms of Use.
  • - Barnes & Noble.com reserves the right not to post any review -- particularly those that do not follow the terms and conditions of these Rules. Barnes & Noble.com also reserves the right to remove any review at any time without notice.
  • - See Terms of Use for other conditions and disclaimers.
Search for Products You'd Like to Recommend

Recommend other products that relate to your review. Just search for them below and share!

Create a Pen Name

Your Pen Name is your unique identity on BN.com. It will appear on the reviews you write and other website activities. Your Pen Name cannot be edited, changed or deleted once submitted.

 
Your Pen Name can be any combination of alphanumeric characters (plus - and _), and must be at least two characters long.

Continue Anonymously

    If you find inappropriate content, please report it to Barnes & Noble
    Why is this product inappropriate?
    Comments (optional)