The Lady Tasting Tea: How Statistics Revolutionized Science in the Twentieth Century

The Lady Tasting Tea: How Statistics Revolutionized Science in the Twentieth Century

by David Salsburg

Paperback(REV)

$19.79 $21.99 Save 10% Current price is $19.79, Original price is $21.99. You Save 10%.
View All Available Formats & Editions
Use Standard Shipping. For guaranteed delivery by December 24, use Express or Expedited Shipping.

Product Details

ISBN-13: 9780805071344
Publisher: Holt, Henry & Company, Inc.
Publication date: 05/01/2002
Edition description: REV
Pages: 352
Sales rank: 410,196
Product dimensions: 5.68(w) x 8.26(h) x 0.91(d)

About the Author

David Salsburg is a retired pharmaceutical company statistician and currently works as a private consultant. He has been a member of the American Statistics Association since 1964 and has taught at Harvard, Connecticut College, the University of Connecticut, the University of Pennsylvania, Rhode Island College, and Trinity College. During his latter years of teaching, Salsburg became Senior Research Fellow at Pfizer, Inc., in the Central Research Department.

Read an Excerpt

The Lady Tasting Tea

How Statistics Revolutionized Science in the Twentieth Century


By David Salsburg

Henry Holt and Company

Copyright © 2001 W. H. Freeman and Company
All rights reserved.
ISBN: 978-1-4668-0178-3



CHAPTER 1

THE LADY TASTING TEA

It was a summer afternoon in Cambridge, England, in the late 1920s. A group of university dons, their wives, and some guests were sitting around an outdoor table for afternoon tea. One of the women was insisting that tea tasted different depending upon whether the tea was poured into the milk or whether the milk was poured into the tea. The scientific minds among the men scoffed at this as sheer nonsense. What could be the difference? They could not conceive of any difference in the chemistry of the mixtures that could exist. A thin, short man, with thick glasses and a Vandyke beard beginning to turn gray, pounced on the problem.

"Let us test the proposition," he said excitedly. He began to outline an experiment in which the lady who insisted there was a difference would be presented with a sequence of cups of tea, in some of which the milk had been poured into the tea and in others of which the tea had been poured into the milk.

I can just hear some of my readers dismissing this effort as a minor bit of summer afternoon fluff. "What difference does it make whether the lady could tell one infusion from another?" they will ask. "There is nothing important or of great scientific merit in this problem," they will sneer. "These great minds should have been putting their immense brain power to something that would benefit mankind."

Unfortunately, whatever nonscientists may think about science and its importance, my experience has been that most scientists engage in their research because they are interested in the results and because they get intellectual excitement out of the work. Seldom do good scientists think about the eventual importance of their work. So it was that sunny summer afternoon in Cambridge. The lady might or might not have been correct about the tea infusion. The fun would be in finding a way to determine if she was right, and, under the direction of the man with the Vandyke beard, they began to discuss how they might make that determination.

Enthusiastically, many of them joined with him in setting up the experiment. Within a few minutes, they were pouring different patterns of infusion in a place where the lady could not see which cup was which. Then, with an air of finality, the man with the Vandyke beard presented her with her first cup. She sipped for a minute and declared that it was one where the milk had been poured into the tea. He noted her response without comment and presented her with the second cup. ...


THE COOPERATIVE NATURE OF SCIENCE

I heard this story in the late 1960s from a man who had been there that afternoon. He was Hugh Smith, but he published his scientific papers under the name H. Fairfield Smith. When I knew him, he was a professor of statistics at the University of Connecticut, in Storrs. I had received my Ph.D. in statistics from the University of Connecticut two years before. After teaching at the University of Pennsylvania, I had joined the clinical research department at Pfizer, Inc., a large pharmaceutical firm. Its research campus in Groton, Connecticut, was about an hour's drive from Storrs. I was dealing with many difficult mathematical problems at Pfizer. I was the only statistician there at that time, and I needed to talk over these problems and my "solutions" to them.

What I had discovered working at Pfizer was that very little scientific research can be done alone. It usually requires a combination of minds. This is because it is so easy to make mistakes. When I would propose a mathematical formula as a means of solving a problem, the model would sometimes be inappropriate, or I might have introduced an assumption about the situation that was not true, or the "solution" I found might have been derived from the wrong branch of an equation, or I might even have made a mistake in arithmetic.

Whenever I would visit the university at Storrs to talk things over with Professor Smith, or whenever I would sit around and discuss problems with the chemists or pharmacologists at Pfizer, the problems I brought out would usually be welcomed. They would greet these discussions with enthusiasm and interest. What makes most scientists interested in their work is usually the excitement of working on a problem. They look forward to the interactions with others as they examine a problem and try to understand it.


THE DESIGN OF EXPERIMENTS

And so it was that summer afternoon in Cambridge. The man with the Vandyke beard was Ronald Aylmer Fisher, who was in his late thirties at the time. He would later be knighted Sir Ronald Fisher. In 1935, he wrote a book entitled The Design of Experiments, and he described the experiment of the lady tasting tea in the second chapter of that book. In his book, Fisher discusses the lady and her belief as a hypothetical problem. He considers the various ways in which an experiment might be designed to determine if she could tell the difference. The problem in designing the experiment is that, if she is given a single cup of tea, she has a 50 percent chance of guessing correctly which infusion was used, even if she cannot tell the difference. If she is given two cups of tea, she still might guess correctly. In fact, if she knew that the two cups of tea were each made with a different infusion, one guess could be completely right (or completely wrong).

Similarly, even if she could tell the difference, there is some chance that she might have made a mistake, that one of the cups was not mixed as well or that the infusion was made when the tea was not hot enough. She might be presented with a series of ten cups and correctly identify only nine of them, even if she could tell the difference.

In his book, Fisher discusses the various possible outcomes of such an experiment. He describes how to decide how many cups should be presented and in what order and how much to tell the lady about the order of presentations. He works out the probabilities of different outcomes, depending upon whether the lady is or is not correct. Nowhere in this discussion does he indicate that such an experiment was ever run. Nor does he describe the outcome of an actual experiment.

The book on experimental design by Fisher was an important element in a revolution that swept through all fields of science in the first half of the twentieth century. Long before Fisher came on the scene, scientific experiments had been performed for hundreds of years. In the later part of the sixteenth century, the English physician William Harvey experimented with animals, blocking the flow of blood in different veins and arteries, trying to trace the circulation of blood as it flowed from the heart to the lungs, back to the heart, out to the body, and back to the heart again.

Fisher did not discover experimentation as a means of increasing knowledge. Until Fisher, experiments were idiosyncratic to each scientist. Good scientists would be able to construct experiments that produced new knowledge. Lesser scientists would often engage in "experimentation" that accumulated much data but was useless for increasing knowledge. An example of this can be seen in the many inconclusive attempts that were made during the late nineteenth century to measure the speed of light. It was not until the American physicist Albert Michelson constructed a highly sophisticated series of experiments with light and mirrors that the first good estimates were made.

In the nineteenth century, scientists seldom published the results of their experiments. Instead, they described their conclusions and published data that "demonstrated" the truth of those conclusions. Gregor Mendel did not show the results of all his experiments in breeding peas. He described the sequence of experiments and then wrote: "The first ten members of both series of experiments may serve as an illustration. ..." (In the 1940s, Ronald Fisher examined Mendel's "illustrations" of data and discovered that the data were too good to be true. They did not display the degree of randomness that should have occurred.)

Although science has been developed from careful thought, observations, and experiments, it was never quite clear how one should go about experimenting, nor were the complete results of experiments usually presented to the reader.

This was particularly true for agricultural research in the late nineteenth and early twentieth centuries. The Rothamsted Agricultural Experimental Station, where Fisher worked during the early years of the twentieth century, had been experimenting with different fertilizer components (called "artificial manures") for almost ninety years before he arrived. In a typical experiment, the workers would spread a mixture of phosphate and nitrogen salts over an entire field, plant grain, and measure the size of the harvest, along with the amount of rainfall during that summer. There were elaborate formulas used to "adjust" the output of one year or one field, in order to compare it to the output of another field or of the same field in another year. These were called "fertility indexes," and each agricultural experimental station had its own fertility index, which it believed was more accurate than any other.

The result of these ninety years of experimentation was a mess of confusion and vast troves of unpublished and useless data. It seemed as if some strains of wheat responded better than other strains to one fertilizer, but only in years when rainfall was excessive. Other experiments seemed to show that sulfate of potash one year, followed by sulfate of soda for the next year, produced an increase in some varieties of potatoes but not others. The most that could be said of these artificial manures was that some of them worked sometimes, perhaps, or maybe.

Fisher, a consummate mathematician, looked at the fertility index that the agricultural scientists at Rothamsted used to correct the results of experiments to account for differences due to the weather from year to year. He examined the competing indexes used by other agricultural experimental stations. When reduced to their elemental algebra, they were all versions of the same formula. In other words, two indexes, whose partisans were hotly contending, were really making exactly the same correction. In 1921, he published a paper in the leading agricultural journal, the Annals of Applied Biology, in which he showed that it did not make any difference what index was used. The article also showed that all these corrections were inadequate to adjust for differences in the fertility of different fields. This remarkable paper ended over twenty years of scientific dispute.

Fisher then examined the data on rainfall and crop production over the previous ninety years and showed that the effects of different weather from year to year were far greater than any effect of different fertilizers. To use a word Fisher developed later in his theory of experimental design, the year-to-year differences in weather and the year-to-year differences in artificial manures were "confounded." This means that there was no way to pull them apart using data from these experiments. Ninety years of experimentation and over twenty years of scientific dispute had been an almost useless waste of effort!

This set Fisher thinking about experiments and experimental design. He concluded that the scientist needs to start with a mathematical model of the outcome of the potential experiment. A mathematical model is a set of equations, in which some of the symbols stand for numbers that will be collected as data from the experiments and other symbols stand for the overall outcomes of the experiment. The scientist starts with the data from the experiment and computes outcomes appropriate to the scientific question being considered.

Consider a simple example from the experience of a teacher with a particular student. The teacher is interested in finding some measure of how much the child has learned. To this end, the teacher "experiments" by giving the child a group of tests. Each test is marked on a scale from 0 to 100. Any one test provides a poor estimate of how much the child knows. It may be that the child did not study the few things that were on that test but knows a great deal about things that were not on the test. The child may have had a headache the day she took a particular test. The child may have had an argument with parents the morning of a particular test. For many reasons, one test does not provide a good estimate of knowledge. So, the teacher gives a set of tests. The average score from all those tests is taken as a better estimate of how much the child knows. How much the child knows is the outcome. The scores on individual tests are the data.

How should the teacher structure those tests? Should they be a sequence of tests that cover only the material taught over the past couple of days? Should they each involve something from all the material taught until now? Should the tests be given weekly, or daily, or at the end of each unit being taught? All of these are questions involved in the design of the experiment.

When the agricultural scientist wants to know the effect of a particular artificial fertilizer on the growth of wheat, an experiment has to be constructed that will provide data to estimate that effect. Fisher showed that the first step in the design of that experiment is to set up a group of mathematical equations describing the relationship between the data that will be collected and the outcomes that are being estimated. Then, any useful experiment has to be one that allows for estimation of those outcomes. The experiment has to be specific and enable the scientist to determine the difference in outcome that is due to weather versus the difference that is due to the use of different fertilizers. In particular, it is necessary to include all the treatments being compared in the same experiment, something that came to be called "controls."

In his book, The Design of Experiments, Fisher provided a few examples of good experimental designs, and derived general rules for good designs. However, the mathematics involved in Fisher's methods were very complicated, and most scientists were unable to generate their own designs unless they followed the pattern of one of the designs Fisher derived in his book.

Agricultural scientists recognized the great value of Fisher's work on experimental design, and Fisherian methods were soon dominating schools of agriculture in most of the English-speaking world. Taking off from Fisher's initial work, an entire body of scientific literature has developed to describe different experimental designs. These designs have been applied to fields other than agriculture, including medicine, chemistry, and industrial quality control. In many cases, the mathematics involved are deep and complicated. But, for the moment, let us stop with the idea that the scientist cannot just go off and "experiment." It takes some long and careful thought—and often a strong dose of difficult mathematics.

And the lady tasting tea, what happened to her? Fisher does not describe the outcome of the experiment that sunny summer afternoon in Cambridge. But Professor Smith told me that the lady identified every single one of the cups correctly.

CHAPTER 2

THE SKEW DISTRIBUTIONS

As with many revolutions in human thought, it is difficult to find the exact moment when the idea of a statistical model became part of science. One can find possible specific examples of it in the work of the German and French mathematicians of the early nineteenth century, and there is even a hint of it in the papers of Johannes Kepler, the great seventeenth-century astronomer. As indicated in the preface to this book, Laplace invented what he called the error function to account for statistical problems in astronomy. I would prefer to date the statistical revolution to the work of Karl Pearson in the 1890s. Charles Darwin recognized biological variation as a fundamental aspect of life and made it the basis of his theory of the survival of the fittest. But it was his fellow Englishman Karl Pearson who first recognized the underlying nature of statistical models and how they offered something different from the deterministic view of nineteenth-century science.

When I began the study of mathematical statistics in the 1960s, Pearson was seldom mentioned in my classes. As I met and talked with the major figures in the field, I heard no references to Pearson or his work. He was either ignored or treated as a minor figure whose activities had long since been outmoded. Churchill Eisenhart, from the U.S. National Bureau of Standards, for instance, was studying at University College, London, during the final years of Karl Pearson's life. He remembered Pearson as a dispirited old man. The pace of statistical research had swept him by, dashing him and most of his work into the dustbin of the past. The bright young students at University College were flocking to study at the feet of the newer great men, one of them Karl Pearson's own son, but no one was coming to see old Karl in his lonely office far from the bustle of new, exciting research.


(Continues...)

Excerpted from The Lady Tasting Tea by David Salsburg. Copyright © 2001 W. H. Freeman and Company. Excerpted by permission of Henry Holt and Company.
All rights reserved. No part of this excerpt may be reproduced or reprinted without permission in writing from the publisher.
Excerpts are provided by Dial-A-Book Inc. solely for the personal use of visitors to this web site.

Table of Contents

Author's PrefaceVII
Chapter 1The Lady Tasting Tea1
Chapter 2The Skew Distributions9
Chapter 3That Dear Mr. Gosset25
Chapter 4Raking Over the Muck Heap33
Chapter 5"Studies in Crop Variation"41
Chapter 6"The Hundred-Year Flood"53
Chapter 7Fisher Triumphant61
Chapter 8The Dose that Kills73
Chapter 9The Bell-Shaped Curve83
Chapter 10Testing the Goodness of Fit93
Chapter 11Hypothesis Testing107
Chapter 12The Confidence Trick117
Chapter 13The Bayesian Heresy125
Chapter 14The Mozart of Mathematics137
Chapter 15The Worm's-Eye View151
Chapter 16Doing Away with Parameters161
Chapter 17When Part is Better than the Whole169
Chapter 18Does Smoking Cause Cancer?181
Chapter 19If You Want the Best Person...195
Chapter 20Just a Plain Texas Farm Boy207
Chapter 21A Genius in the Family217
Chapter 22The Picasso of Statistics229
Chapter 23Dealing with Contamination237
Chapter 24The Man Who Remade Industry247
Chapter 25Advice from the Lady in Black257
Chapter 26The March of the Martingales267
Chapter 27The Intent to Treat275
Chapter 28The Computer Turns upon Itself285
Chapter 29The Idol with Feet of Clay293
Afterword311
Timeline313
Bibliography317
Index327

Customer Reviews

Most Helpful Customer Reviews

See All Customer Reviews

The Lady Tasting Tea: How Statistics Revolutionized Science in the Twentieth Century 3.3 out of 5 based on 0 ratings. 4 reviews.
rivkat on LibraryThing 19 days ago
A bunch of mini biographies of statisticians, along with sort-of descriptions of why what they did was important. I say sort-of because there was often not enough for me to really understand why what they did was important¿I will certainly accept that the difference between parametric and nonparametric statistics is vital and has real-world implications, but this isn¿t the book to explain why, and maybe such a book would have to be a textbook. To me, the book hung awkwardly between ¿popular¿ and ¿science,¿ and I would have liked more on the latter.
neurodrew on LibraryThing 20 days ago
This book is a chatty, nonmathematical account of the evolution of statistics as an approach to science in the 20th century. Most of the characters are English. The title refers to an incident of an improptu experiment set up by R.A. Fisher at a garden party when a lady claimed to be able to detect if tea had milk added or if the milk had been poured first. There is no record of the outcome. The book detailed numerous conflicts between Karl Pearson, R.A. Fisher, and Jerzy Neyman at the University of London. There was a politically correct nod to the inclusion of women, including Florence Nightengale. The book was well written but would have benefited from more mathematics.
sgerbic on LibraryThing 20 days ago
Reviewed Aug 2002 This is a math history book for non-math people, I found it really interesting in parts and dry/detailed in others. Salsburg writes well and explains bits of the lives behind the people involved in statistics. He tries to include women as much as possible but leaves out minorities completely. (are their many to include?) I am interested to learn that Florence Nightingale invented pie charts. She used them to explain that more men die from illness during war than in action. A great quote, "Although it has been studied for about 2,500 years, Aristotle's codification of logic is a relativity useless tool." The part about management setting unrealistic expatiations using red and white beads was great. And Stella Canfiffe who said, "It is no use as statisticians, our being sniffy about the slapdash methods of many sociologists unless we are prepared to try them into more scientifically acceptable thought." John Tukey explains tally marks for counting are useless (llll llll) it is too easy to make a mistake. He suggests using 10 as the base and making dots, lines and crosses. 14-2002
Anonymous More than 1 year ago
This is a very interesting and readable account of many of the important developers of statistics and the application of statistics. Many of those whose names are given to particular test statistics come to life. The author does a very good job of referring to some of the underlying mathematics used by some researchers without becoming dry or too complex for those with (like me) poor mathematical grounding.